Mom, Dad, DNA and Suicide
It was only in 2004 that scientists led by Michael Meaney of McGill University reported an intriguing study in which the on-off switches on the DNA of baby rats were set by their mother’s behavior. Specifically, they reported, when mother rats licked and groomed their pups, the gene that turns up production of the brain’s glucocorticoid receptor worked at full steam. But when the rat moms were neglectful, the promoter gene was silenced. Result: fewer receptors, and a hair-trigger response to stress. These neglected rats grew up to be fearful, neurotic messes because this gene had a silencer sitting on it.
Ever since that study, the obvious question has hung in the air: Are people like rats? Do childhood experiences re-set the genome, turning some genes on and others off, or turning some to “high production” and some to “low”?
Meany and his team are back with a fascinating new study, in the online edition of Nature Neuroscience, that answers yes. Early childhood abuse can change the expression of the same gene as in rats—the glucocorticoid receptor, which shapes how people respond to stress.
The scientists found that in the brains of people who committed suicide and who had been abused as children, the glucocorticoid receptor gene was dialed way down. It was producing fewer receptors (which are proteins) than was the same gene in suicide victims who had not been abused as children. Even more impressive, the scientists discovered the reason for the low rate of production: a stretch of DNA that acts as a rheostat on the receptor gene (in genetics parlance, the rheostat is a promoter region) had had a silencer slapped on it. The silencer is a methyl group, the exact same molecular entity that had silenced the receptor gene in the rats.
Such changes are called “epigenetic,” to distinguish them from changes that affect the sequence of nucleotides in DNA. Epigenetics is arguably the next frontier in genetic research, promising to show why people with identical DNA, such as monozygotic twins, have different traits, including traits known to be strongly affected by genes. The answer seems to be that the events of our lives, including parental behavior, turns some genes on and some genes off. In this case, parental care (or, specifically, abuse) changed the expression of the crucial glucocorticoid-receptor gene in the brain.
When I met Meaney at a 2004 conference that got neuroscientists together with the Dalai Lama, he was truly eloquent about the import of such research, as I tried to convey in my last book. For too long we have been in the grip of genetic determinism, the idea that the genes we enter the world with shape the lives we lead, that genes have behavior on a short leash. But as Meaney pointed out, that is not only defeatist—ceding responsibility for who we are and how we act to forces beyond our control—but scientifically wrong. The lives we lead reach into the very double helices of our cells.